Which type of synchronicity has a lag between receiving and responding to a message?

This is a preview. Log in through your library.

Abstract

This paper expands, refines, and explicates media synchronicity theory, originally proposed in a conference proceeding in 1999 (Dennis and Valacich 1999). Media synchronicity theory (MST) focuses on the ability of media to support synchronicity, a shared pattern of coordinated behavior among individuals as they work together. We expand on the original propositions of MST to argue that communication is composed of two primary processes: conveyance and convergence. The familiarity of individuals with the tasks they are performing and with their coworkers will also affect the relative amounts of these two processes. Media synchronicity theory proposes that for conveyance processes, use of media supporting lower synchronicity should result in better communication performance. For convergence processes, use of media supporting higher synchronicity should result in better communication performance. We identify five capabilities of media (symbol sets, parallelism, transmission velocity, rehearsability, and reprocessability) that influence the development of synchronicity and thus the successful performance of conveyance and convergence communication processes. The successful completion of most tasks involving more than one individual requires both conveyance and convergence processes, thus communication performance will be improved when individuals use a variety of media to perform a task, rather than just one medium.

Journal Information

The editorial objective of the MIS Quarterly is the enhancement and communication of knowledge concerning the development of IT-based services, the management of IT resources, and the use, impact, and economics of IT with managerial, organizational, and societal implications. Professional issues affecting the IS field as a whole are also in the purview of the journal.

Publisher Information

Established in 1968, the University of Minnesota Management Information Systems Research Center promotes research in MIS topics by bridging the gap between the corporate and academic MIS worlds through the events in the MISRC Associates Program.

  • Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

  • Laura Marciano,
  • Anna Maria Annoni,
  • Anne-Linda Camerini

“What you say and how you say it” matters: An experimental evidence of the role of synchronicity, modality, and message valence during smartphone-mediated communication

  • Serena Petrocchi, 
  • Laura Marciano, 
  • Anna Maria Annoni, 
  • Anne-Linda Camerini

Which type of synchronicity has a lag between receiving and responding to a message?

x

  • Published: September 17, 2020
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237846

Figures

Abstract

Nowadays, smartphone-Mediated Communication (SMC) has become a popular form of social interactions. The present experimental study manipulated three aspects of messaging in a WhatsApp chat as a form of SMC: synchronicity (immediate vs. time-lagged response), modality (with or without emojis), and valence (empathic accurate vs. empathic inaccurate response). The aim of this study was to investigate whether these three aspects had an impact on perceived social support, interpersonal trust, and personality attribution of the communication partner. The partial mediation of perceived social presence (the evaluation of the communication partner’s accessibility) and subjective social presence (the perception of being concordant with him/her) was also examined. Participants were 160 young adults, balanced in gender. They were randomly assigned to different the experimental conditions where they engaged in a manipulated WhatsApp chat with a fictitious same-gender communication partner. Post-questionnaire data were analyzed using Structural Equation Modeling. Message valence (empathic accurate response) and modality (with emojis) significantly predicted higher levels of both forms of social presence. Synchronicity (immediate response) predicted higher levels of perceived but not subjective social presence. Social presence, in turn, was positively associated with social support, while subjective, but not perceived social presence, was positively associated with personality attribution. Neither perceived nor subjective social presence were related to interpersonal trust. Our results show that both what is said and how it is said impact the experience of interpersonal relations in SMC.

Citation: Petrocchi S, Marciano L, Annoni AM, Camerini A-L (2020) “What you say and how you say it” matters: An experimental evidence of the role of synchronicity, modality, and message valence during smartphone-mediated communication. PLoS ONE 15(9): e0237846. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237846

Editor: Frantisek Sudzina, Aalborg University, DENMARK

Received: April 18, 2020; Accepted: August 4, 2020; Published: September 17, 2020

Copyright: © 2020 Petrocchi et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding: ALC and SP received financial support for this work by the Swiss National Foundation under the Grant Number 10DL1C_183199. The funder has no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests: no authors have competing interests.

Introduction

Nowadays, the smartphone has become a ubiquitous device in our daily lives, especially among younger populations. In Switzerland, 79% of the population owned a smartphone in 2020. In 2021, the estimated number of smartphone users in the country will reach 7.33 million. Instant messaging and social networking show strong growth as well. In 2019, 2.95 billion people worldwide used smartphone-messaging apps, 2.15 million more compared to the previous year [1].

Although smartphones offer a wide range of functionalities, instant messaging remains among the most popular and used ones [2]. Due to the possibility to use text-based conversations complemented by visual elements such as emojis or images, instant messaging offers a particularly rich way of one-to-one or group communication. However, the dislocation of time and space during the conversation remains a different important characteristic compared to face-to-face communication. Thus, it is not surprising that Smartphone-Mediated Communication (SMC) has received attention by researchers aiming to understand how this form of interaction shapes human relationships (e.g., [3–7]).

Although past research has investigated the differential effects of online and offline communication on psychosocial functioning [8–10], SMC is a fast-changing environment in which technological advancements spread quickly and widely in the population. Nowadays, Internet access on the mobile phone is overtaking home broadband services [11]. Alongside free-to-use instant messenger services such as WhatsApp or Telegram are surpassing mobile SMS and MMS services [12], potentially contributing to the evolution of interpersonal relationships.

The portability of the smartphone, the perception of its indispensability for everyday activities [13], and its predominant use in the context of social interactions and interpersonal communication [14] make smartphone-mediated communication (SMC) and its effect on interpersonal functioning a crucial point to be studied. The meta-analysis by Liu and Chang [15] summarizes the differences in interpersonal closeness across different communication channels, including SMC and instant messaging. Results show that mobile phone-based channels are predominantly used to stay in contact with close friends and that instant messaging is related to friendship quality [16, 17]. What is not yet well known is what are the characteristics of SMC that influence and transform the subjective experience during mediated interactions. This experimental study aims to contribute to scientific evidence on the role of specific aspects of SMC (i.e. synchronicity, modality, and message valence) in the perception of the communication partner and the quality of the interpersonal relationship in the context of SMC. In particular, we focused on the construct of social presence to capture how a communication partner is perceived during an interaction. The quality of the interpersonal relationship has been explicated in terms of perceived interpersonal trust, social support, and attribution of personality characteristics to the communication partner. Before introducing the study, the scientific literature on SMC and related concepts will be reviewed to derive a theoretical model guiding the experimental study and the interpretation of its results.

Smartphone-mediated communication and social presence

Compared to face-to-face interactions, SMC has specific features that have an impact on the perception of social presence, which, in turn, may influence the evaluation of the other (e.g., personality attribution) and the quality of the interaction itself [18]. First, SMC is characterized by the dislocation of time and space, since it allows to interact both in a synchronous and asynchronous way, in a constant-connected environment [19]. However, little is known about how synchronicity affects the individual’s perception of the quality of the social interaction via SMC.

A second characteristic of the SMC is its lack of physical, visual, and paralinguistic cues derived from body language and speech intonation. However, principles of Media Richness Theory indicate that the possibility to use non- and para-verbal cues, such as emojis or images, can enhance the message value and mimic the natural language of the communicator [20–22]. The content of the message itself is not a specific feature of the SMC, neither is the valence of the message (positive or negative). However, both could have a potential impact on the perception of the quality of the social interaction via SMC, especially if considered in combination with synchronicity and the use of non- and para-verbal cues.

In SMC, one crucial aspect is how people perceive the other person in the interaction as being “real” or as being “here” [23, 24]. When communication happens through digital devices, this sensation is associated with perceived social presence in human relationships [7, 25]. According to Biocca, Harms, & Burgoon [26], the concept of social presence can be divided into three dimensions. Co-presence, defined as an automatic state, consists in the perception of a coherent source of inputs from an interlocutor classified as an agent [26]. Perceived social presence of the other, involving an interpretative process of the psychological and behavioral engagement, leads to the evaluation of the other’s accessibility, i.e. how distant the other is perceived [26]. Finally, subjective social presence is the perception of being concordant with the other, resulting in a feeling of connectedness and symmetry within the relationship [26]. All three dimensions are interconnected and activated in the mediated communication process. To note, perceptions of social presence largely depend on the synchronicity of the interaction, as long pauses in the mediated communication process decrease perceptions of “being there” as well as the awareness and imagination of the other person [27]. Thus, our first hypothesis was:

HP1: Synchronous (vs. asynchronous) SMC increases perceptions of social presence, both at the perceptual and subjective level.

Furthermore, modality (e.g., the use of verbal and visual cues) determines the experience of social presence [7]. For example, non-verbal cues such as emoticons and emojis can compensate the lack of social context signs and enhance the emotional value of the message [22, 28], leading to higher levels of social presence [29, 30]. In fact, emoticons, originally created using standard text characters [31], are surrogates for non-verbal emotional expression [32]. Nowadays, they are represented by small icons, called emojis, such as smiley or sad faces, embedded in the text message. Emojis are considered the evolution of emoticons, as more expressive and more capable of conveying emotional messages [33]. They strengthen the content of the message [34] and disambiguate the communicational intent, providing information about the personality of the interlocutor [35]. Further advancements can be witnessed in Apple’s introduction of memoj stickers for the iPhone in autumn 2019. These stickers are customizable animojis for the users, who can define face shape, skin, eyes color, eyewear, and hairstyle to create an animated representation of themselves. According to Derks, Fischer, & Bos [36], thanks to such cues, the differences between the online communication of emotions compared to the face-to-face one almost disappear. Hence, we hypothesized that:

HP2: Text messages with emojis (vs. text only) in SMC increase perceptions of social presence, both at the perceptual and subjective level.

Aside from synchronicity and modality, as previously mentioned, the valence of the message content needs to be considered. Social presence comes with the expression of emotions, feelings, and mood [37], which contribute to the psychological involvement of the interlocutor [26]. One element, which has been found to be relevant in the experience of social presence, is the empathic accuracy of the interlocutor [38–40]. Empathic accuracy is defined as the degree to which a person infers what other people think and feel [41, 42], enhancing the experience of positive or negative feelings and emotions. In this vein, an empathic accurate answer to a request is a message in which the sender takes into consideration the receiver’s mental state (i.e., what he/she thinks and how he/she feels) and replies accordingly, thus creating a positive experience. Indeed, empathic accuracy plays a vital role in relationship satisfaction [43] and conflict resolution [44]. In turn, social presence is affected by the emotional responses generated by the mediated communication, and it is triggered by empathic or aversive reactions to the other [26, 45]. Accordingly, we hypothesized that:

HP3: Messages with positive valence, i.e. empathic accuracy (vs. negative valence, i.e. empathic inaccuracy) increase perceptions of social presence, both at the perceptual and subjective level.

Social presence and perceived trust, social support, and personality of the communication partner

Due to the ongoing digitalization process, trust has been in the focus of researchers and the general public alike as in times of fake news and online grooming (to name two examples) people are more and more concerned with the trustworthiness of online sources and information [46]. Trust can be defined as the expectation held by an individual that another person maintains their words, promises, verbal or written statements [47]. Over time, trust becomes a stable personality characteristic [47, 48]. At the same time, trust is a state characteristic depending on the target person and the content of the communication [49–51]. Trust can equally emerge during online interactions [50, 52–54]. In this vein, social presence has been reported to increase online trust [55, 56]; hence, in online and collaborative contexts [57], social presence has been considered an indispensable prerequisite for the development of interpersonal trust [58]. Additionally, also empathic accuracy (vs. empathic inaccuracy) has been found to have an influence on online trust towards another person [59].

Trust is an important ingredient for the development and maintenance of well-functioning relationships [60–62]. It is tightly linked to social support, defined as the propensity to perceive the other person in a relationship as supportive, reliable, and able to give help and guidance [63]. In particular, being able to establish a connection with the other in a collaborative environment based on empathic responses [39] allows to build a perception of social support and intimacy [64].

Another aspect affected by social presence is the personality attribution to the conversation partner during SMC. Similar to face-to-face evaluations, text-based and online communications arise the formation of first impressions about others’ personalities [65], reflecting their online behaviors [67, 68]. Adults use personality traits to describe themselves and others [69], and to interpret the others’ behaviors, based on attributional schemes, with different levels of accuracy or attributional styles [70]. The development of the perception of others’ personality (e.g., impression formation), is a process involving both automatic and conscious components [71], also based on brief interactions and nonverbal behaviors [72, 73]. The impression formation, however, is limited, context-dependent and highly vulnerable to patterns and biases [74], especially in the earliest stages of an interaction [75]. In particular, people generally tend to rely on specific attributes, which can be summarized by the interpersonal traits of agreeableness, neuroticism and extraversion [76, 77], which are the most used during the personality attribution process [78]. Hence, we state our last hypothesis as follows:

HP4: Social presence partially mediates the relations between SMC and interpersonal trust, social support, and personality attribution.

Study objectives

Using an experimental design, this study aims to extend the scientific evidence considering the role of specific aspects of SMC in the perception of the communication partner, the conversation, and the interpersonal relationship in general. In particular, three elements of the SMC were manipulated, i.e. synchronicity, modality, and message valence, to investigate whether they have an impact on interpersonal trust, social support, and personality attribution, through the mediation of social presence. Considered previous findings from Park et al. [7], we manipulated SMC by introducing synchronicity delays (high vs. low) in the communication process, and visual elements (text vs. text and emojis). Furthermore, inspired by the textual stimuli introduced by Feng et al. [59] in a study on empathic accuracy and interpersonal trust, we manipulated the valence of the message (positive empathic accurate vs. negative empathic inaccurate). Fig 1 shows the theoretical model tested.

Materials and methods

Experimental study design

An experimental study, conducted in a laboratory setting, was used to manipulate one-to-one SMC interaction. Participants were presented with a specific imaginary scenario, in which they just moved in a new city where they did not yet know anyone except for a colleague, equal in age and gender. They got the phone number of this colleague to ask his/her help for some favors they needed. The scenarios were gender adapted. Female participants communicated with a peer called Sophie, while male participants communicated with a peer called Mark. The decision to use same-gender interactions was based on two reasons: First, there is evidence in the literature showing that females and males show different communication styles during mediated communication (e.g., [79]) and that they have different aims during their smartphone interactions (e.g., [80]). To avoid any possible influence of gender, we decided to match the gender of study participants and the fictitious peer. Second, we wanted to avoid any romantic connotations. Dependent on the experimental condition, the peer replied either with an empathic accurate or an empathic inaccurate message (i.e., positive vs. negative valence), with or without emojis (i.e., high vs. low richness of modality), and immediately after the participant sent the request or 7 minutes later (i.e., synchronicity vs. asychronicity). The study received ethical approval by the Università della Svizzera italiana Institutional Review Board (approval date 30/10/2018).

Development and evaluation of experimental material

Before conducting the main experiment, we performed a pilot study to develop and evaluate the experimental material to the specific context of Canton Ticino (Italian-speaking Switzerland), where the study was conducted. The pilot study focused on the identification of which favors and responses could best fit the balance between a scientific setting and the ecological component of the proposed interactions. Participants in the pilot study were 107 young adults (Mage = 26.05, SD = 3.97; 67.3% female). A first set of twelve different favors was defined. Every favor was evaluated with respect to plausibility (“how realistic is the requested favor to you?”), frequency (“how many times did you find yourself in this situation, as the person in need or the one providing help?”), time and effort required to satisfy the request (“in your opinion, how much time/effort is required in order to satisfy this favor?”). A 5-points Likert scale was employed to rate the four dimensions, ranging from 1 = “Not at all/Never” to 5 = “Totally/A lot”. The selection criteria were: 1) higher summed score of the means across the above-mentioned dimensions (plausibility, frequency, time, and effort), where plausibility was double-weighted to preserve the ecological validity, and 2) differentiation to avoid similar requests (e.g., mother tongue lessons before an interview vs. adjustment of CV). Three favors were finally selected: (1) ask Sophie/Mark to offer lessons in her/his mother tongue before an interview, (2) help with a work/study project, and (3) help moving a couch.

Six empathic and six non-empathic responses were evaluated from the most negative to the most positive on a 100-point visual analogue scale, ranging from -50 = “Very negative” to +50 = “Very positive”. The highest three for each polarity were adapted to the help-related contexts and embedded in the experiment. The detailed results of the pilot study are available upon request.

A set of popular emojis, equally split between positive and negative, was selected by using the emojis tracker for Twitter (https://www.emojitracker.com/), as done in previous studies [81, 82]. For empathic emojis, the following ones were selected: “Smiling face with smiling eyes”, “Smiling face with sunglasses”, and “Smiling face with open mouth and smiling eyes”, while both “Angry faces” (both yellow and red) and the “Unamused face” were chosen for the non-empathic responses. S1 Fig includes examples of the manipulated messages.

Experimental study procedure

Participants were recruited through Facebook posts, flyers, and snowball sampling. The study focused on young adults, as they are particularly active on WhatsApp [11]. Only participants between 18 and 35 years of age, fluent in Italian, were eligible for the experimental study. A quota sampling strategy was applied to assure a balanced distribution of gender. Prior to the start of the experimental session, participants received a brief introduction addressing potential ethical concerns, and they signed the informed consent.

First, participants had to complete a questionnaire assessing socio-demographic characteristics. Next, participants received a smartphone, with printed information, describing the previously developed gender-matched scenario: they were new in the city and got the number of Mark (for male participants) or Sophie (for female participants). The participants received the instruction to contact the peer through WhatsApp (installed on the smartphone device) and ask him/her three favors from a list provided by the researchers. The necessary contact details were already saved in the smartphone. After participants briefly introduced themselves (“Hi Sophie/Mark, I am [participant’s name]. We exchanged our phone numbers a few days ago”), they asked the first of three favors. To increase the ecological validity of the experiment, they could choose the order of the favors. The peer (Mark or Sophie) was a researcher located in another room using WhatsApp (on a dual SIM smartphone) to simulate the male character (with the associated phone number) or the female character (with the associated phone number). Based on the randomly assigned condition, the participants received one of eight manipulated answer options (see S1 Fig for examples).

Between each of the three favor-answer-interaction, participants saw a 2-minutes video with images of peaceful landscapes that functioned as a neutral stimulus. After the experimental phase with the three favor-answer-interaction was over, the participants received a final questionnaire assessing post-test variables. At the end of the experiment, they received a short debriefing and 30 Swiss Francs for compensation. The flowchart in S2 Fig summarizes the experimental study procedure.

Experimental study measures

All measures were self-report and, if available, validated scales were used. Forward and backward translations from English to Italian and vice versa by two independent translators were implied to assure linguistic validity. If necessary, scales were adapted to the adult context (e.g., items referring to the school environment were adapted to college and/or work situations). Measures were assessed via an online questionnaire in Qualtrics, accessible on a computer in the laboratory where the experiment was carried out. Within a scale, the item order was randomized, whereas the order of the scales was predetermined.

Perceived social presence.

The perceived component of social presence was assessed with a set of semantic differentials based on previous literature [83]. Eight bipolarized adjectives were chosen to address the social presence of the peer. Participants were asked to evaluate the peer as being: 1) sensitive–insensitive, 2) warm–cold, 3) responsive–non-responsive, 4) interactive–non-interactive, 5) near–distant, 6) personal–impersonal, 7) authentic–superficial, and 8) sociable–non-sociable. Answer options ranged from 1 to 7. All scores were averaged, with higher scores reflecting a higher level of perceived social presence. Cronbach’s Alpha was good, α = .87, as well as the inter-item correlations, r > .47. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) revealed that the one-factor tested model reached good fit of the data, χ2 (19) = 35.634, p = .012, SRMR = .0434, RMSEA = .074, CFI = .978.

Subjective social presence.

The self-report measure developed by Burgoon and Hale [84] was applied to assess subjective social presence. A total of eleven items, belonging to Similarity/Receptivity/Inclusion, Non-Immediacy, and Formality subscales, were employed and adapted to the context of the experiment. Measured on a Likert scale, response options ranged from 1 = “Strongly agree” to 7 = “Strongly disagree”. Example items are: “Mark/Sophie tried to establish a relationship” (Similarity/Receptivity/Inclusion subscale), “Mark/Sophie was very detached” (non-immediacy subscale), and “Mark/Sophie was not responsive to my ideas” (formality subscale). A total score was obtained by averaging single item scores, with higher scores corresponding to higher levels of subjective social presence. Cronbach’s Alpha was good, α = .89, as well as the inter-item correlations, r > .41. CFA revealed that the one-factor tested model reached good fit of the data, χ2 (134) = 220.960, p < .001, SRMR = .0566, RMSEA = .064, CFI = .953.

Interpersonal trust.

The original Trust in Close Relationships scale [49] was employed to assess interpersonal trust in the dyadic communicative exchange. Due to the loving-related and intimate nature of the predictability dimension, the related items referred to this domain were removed. A total of 18 items, belonging to Faith and Dependability original subscales, were adapted to the experimental context by weakening the intimate texture of the dimensions. Measured on a Likert scale, response options ranged from 1 = “Strongly disagree” to 7 = “Strongly agree”. Example items are: “I can rely on Mark/Sophie to react in a positive way when I expose my weaknesses to him/her” (Faith subscale) and “Mark/Sophie has proven to be trustworthy and I am willing to let him/her engage in activities which other partners find too threatening” (Dependability subscale). A total score was obtained by averaging single item scores, with higher scores corresponding to higher levels of trust in Mark/Sophie. Cronbach’s Alpha was good, α = .92, as well as the inter-item correlations, r > .40. CFA revealed that the one-factor tested model reached good fit of the data, χ2 (134) = 220.960, p < .001, SRMR = .0566, RMSEA = .064, CFI = .953.

Social support.

Received social support was assessed with the Actually Received Support scale of the Berlin Social Support Scale (BSSS; [85]). The scale structure is oriented on the dyadic supportive behavior and how it is perceived. Given the experimental study design, which was intended to create a needing-help-environment anchored, among others, on time and effort required to satisfy the requests, the informational subscale was excluded. Therefore, the scale was composed by 13 items, referring to emotional support (EMO; 9 items, example item: “Mark/Sophie did not show much empathy for my situation”), instrumental support (INST; 3 items, example item: “Mark/Sophie took care of things I could not manage on my own”) and the final item of satisfaction with support (SAT; “In general, I am very satisfied with the way Mark/Sophie behaved”). Measured on a Likert scale, response options ranged from 1 = “Strongly disagree” to 4 = “Strongly agree”. A total score was obtained by averaging responses, where higher score indicated higher levels of perceived support received from Sophie/Mark. Cronbach’s Alpha was good, α = .97, as well as the inter-item correlations, r > .70. CFA revealed that the one-factor tested model reached good fit of the data, χ2 (50) = 96.367, p < .001, SRMR = .0260, RMSEA = .063, CFI = .978.

Personality attribution.

Perception of marker personality was assessed using three items, capturing the Ashton, Lee and Goldberg [86] HEXACO model of personality. The model foresees six personality dimensions: Honesty/Humility, Emotional stability, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Openness. For the purpose of the present research, Agreeableness, Extraversion, and Emotional stability were chosen as interpersonal traits [76]. Participants were asked to report how much they would describe Mark/Sophie as 1) calm, 2) agreeable, and 3) outgoing. Each adjective was rated on a 7-point Likert scale, with answer options ranging from 1 = “Strongly disagree” to 7 = “Strongly agree”. Cronbach’s Alpha was good, α = .79, as well as the inter-item correlations, r > .56. Higher scores indicated higher levels of the respective personality dimension.

Experimental study participants

A total of 160 young adults took part in the experimental study (Mage = 23.38, SD = 3.99, range 18–35 years old). These participants were different from the sample of the pilot study to evaluate the experimental study material. Participants were balanced in gender (50%, N = 80, male) and they were primarily based in Ticino, Switzerland (88.8%. N = 142), high school graduated (40%, N = 64) or college graduated (38.8%, N = 62). College students represented 65.6% (N = 105) of the sample.

Experimental data analysis

Because a forced-answer format was used for all measures, the data did not include any missing values. Descriptive statistics, bivariate correlations, and regression analyses were performed in SPSS version 25 [87]. The hypothesized model was tested using the lavaan package [88] in R software [89], using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors and a Satorra-Bentler scaled test statistic. The three features of SMC were dummy coded as following: synchronous (1) vs asynchronous (0), modality with emojis (1) vs modality without (0), and positive emphatic valence (1) vs negative non-empathic valence (0).

Results

Correlations between all measures are shown in Table 1. Subjective social presence, perceived social presence, social support, interpersonal trust, and personality attribution were all positively and significantly correlated to each other. The synchronicity and the modality of the message correlated significantly with perceived social presence, whereas the valence of the message correlated significantly with all the other variables.

The hypothesized model (see Fig 1) reached good fit of the data, χ2 (6) = 8.24, p = .22, CFI = .99, SRMR = .012, RMSEA = .048, 90% CI [.000, .119]. Examination of path coefficients yielded evidence for the hypothesized paths, with few exceptions. Fig 2 represents the standardized regression weights of the constructs included in the model.

Fig 2. Final model with social support, interpersonal trust, and personality attribution as dependent variables and subjective and perceived social presence as mediators.

Standardized coefficients are shown. Reference groups: valence (negative, non-empathic), modality (without emojis), synchronicity (7 minutes delay). Dashed arrows denote non-significant paths; all other paths denote significant relationships at * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237846.g002

Social support was predicted by both the subjective (β = .14, p < .01) and perceived social presence (β = .16, p < .01), showing that an increase in social presence corresponds to an increase in social support. Moreover, personality attribution to Sophie/Mark was positively predicted by subjective social presence (β = .43, p < .01) but not by perceived social presence. The two social presence variables were not significantly related to interpersonal trust. Furthermore, subjective and perceived social presence significantly correlated with each other (r = .624, p < .01). Message valence was significantly related to both subjective (β = .78, p < .01) and perceived social presence (β = .62, p < .01), indicating that when the valence of the message was positive (empathic) social presence increased. Likewise, modality was positively related to both subjective (β = .17, p < .01) and perceived social presence (β = .16, p < .01). Furthermore, synchronicity was positively associated with perceived social presence (β = .16, p < .01), but not with subjective social presence.

Moreover, the indirect paths involving subjective social presence as a mediator were significant between valence and social support (β = .11, p < .05) as well as valence and personality (β = .33, p < .01), and modality and personality (β = .07, p < .01). On the other side, the indirect only significant paths involving perceived social presence as a mediator were the ones between synchronicity and social support (β = .03, p = .03), modality and social support (β = .03, p = .02), and, once again, valence and social support (β = .10, p < .01). No significant indirect effects were found for personality attribution (see Table 2 for full details of the model).

Discussion

The portability of the smartphone, the perception of its indispensability for everyday activities [13], and its predominant use in the context of social interactions and interpersonal communication [14] make smartphone-mediated communication (SMC) and its effect on interpersonal functioning a crucial point to be studied.

The present study tested a partial mediation model considering SMC features (i.e., synchronicity, modality, and valence of the message) as predictor, social presence (subjective and perceived) as mediator, and social support, interpersonal trust, and the evaluation of the communication partner’s personality as outcome variables. SMC features were manipulated in a randomized experiment and mediator and outcome variables were measured immediately after the experiment. Based on data from 160 young adults, we found that message valence and modality were both associated with social presence, measured with its perceptual and subjective components. On average, empathic messages and the use of emojis increased the perception of the communication partner as real and present in the SMC conversation. These results confirm findings from previous literature. Indeed, empathic accuracy has been found to increment social presence in mediated communication [41, 42] the elicitation of positive emotions [45] and satisfaction with the relationship [43]. Furthermore, the use of verbal and visual cues has been previously shown to influence the positive experience of social presence [7]. According to the principles of Media Richness Theory, emojis can enhance the message value by mimicking the natural non- and para-verbal language of the communicator [20–22]. That said, the use of emojis in the present study reinforced the meaning of the message and compensated for the lack of non-verbal and para-verbal cues, thus enhancing the emotional value of the message [28], which eventually led to higher levels of social presence [30].

Previous literature stated that long pauses in the mediated communication process decrease perceptions of “being there” as well as the awareness and imagination of the other person [27]. In a similar way, our results indicate that the synchronicity of the message (i.e., without a temporal delay) increases the perceptual component of social presence only, which is the evaluation of the other’s accessibility and the extent of how distant the other is perceived. However, the synchronicity of the message does not affect the subjective social presence, which is the perception of being concordant and symmetric with the other. Concordance relates to agreement, harmony, and responsiveness, which are strongly connected to the message valence and not to the perceived immediacy of the SMC, although non-immediacy was a subscale measured as part of subjective social presence. It seems that the latter is overruled by the other measures of subjective social presence to capture the perception of being concordant with the communication partner. A second explanation for this finding may be that the suggested scenarios were built on different requests for help, which created unbalance in the symmetry in the communication partners.

Finally, it was expected that social presence partially mediates the relations between SMC and interpersonal trust, social support, and personality attribution. Our results show that subjective social presence, indeed, significantly increases the social support received from the communication partner, and it leads to a more positive evaluation of his/her personality. Higher levels of social support are also predicted by perceived social presence. Furthermore, similarly to face-to-face communication, our results demonstrate that SMC allows the formation of first impressions about others’ personalities [66] based on their online behaviors [67, 68]. The formation of first impressions is context-dependent and highly vulnerable to patterns and biases [74], especially in computer/smartphone-mediated communication [90], and it depends on the perception of being concordant with the other.

On the other hand, interpersonal trust is not linked to either subjective or perceived social presence, but it is directly predicted by the valence of the message. Thus, contrary to what emerged in communication research in other online environments (i.e., B2C e-Commerce; [55, 56]), during a conversation that imitates the everyday interactions among peers, it is the empathic vs. non-empathic content of the message that influences the perception of trust. Neither modality nor synchronicity had an impact on trust. Thus, interpersonal trust, as a combination of faith and dependability, may be released from the perception of the other as emotionally concordant and be more related to the ready availability of the other.

Limitations

Despite the experimental design, which allows to make causal claims about the impact of SCM on social presence and the quality of the constructed interpersonal relationship, the present research has several limitations. First, the small sample size does not allow to consider all the interactions between the three experimental conditions in the full model. It would be interesting to analyze whether social presence, and, subsequently, social support, trust, and personality attribution would change according to the combination of synchronicity, modality, and message valence. Second, as for every other experimental study, the ecological validity is limited. Although our pilot study helped to identify scenarios, favors, and emojis young adults can relate to, other aspects could threat the ecological validity. For example, the 7-minutes delay in the asynchronous condition may be somewhat arbitrary and not perceived as asynchronous in real-life SMC interactions. Third, since the measures are all self-reported, we cannot exclude a degree of social desirability. Future research should investigate SMC applying self-report measures in combination with objective measures such as psycho-physiological correlates (e.g., heart rate, electrodermal activity) as done in other contexts of communication and media research (e.g., [91–93]).

Conclusions

The results of the present experimental study provide important answers related to the immediate impact of SMC on people’s personal feelings and subjective experience while interacting with others. In sum, the results underline that synchronous empathic messages with emojis are valid predictors of social presence, which, in turn, is positively linked to social support and personality attribution. Moreover, the attribution of trust to a peer, social support, and personality attribution are directly determined by the empathic accuracy of the message, i.e. when the response matches the request by the individual required support. A proverb says: “It’s not what you say, it’s how you say it.” In our study on SMC, however, it should be rewritten to underline: “It is both what you say and how you say it that makes the difference”.

This conclusion stresses the importance of learning about the implications of our everyday SMC practices, ranging from informal family and peer-to-peer communication to SMC in workplace settings or customer care. Given that SMC already starts in preadolescence, where smartphone ownership has gone up to 90 percent among 11 year-olds [94], learning about the implications on the perceptions and feelings induced by different ways of SMC should be on the agenda both at home but also in school curricula aimed at promoting a conscious use of the smartphone. Such a use is not only beneficial for subjective well-being but also for the initiation and maintenance of good interpersonal relationships.

Besides these practical implications, the present study lays the ground for future experimental research on SMC. In particular, future studies should include experimental observations in natural settings, over time, pay more attention to smartphone addictive behaviors, and consider younger age populations. Since the present study allows to conclude on the short-term impact of SMC only, this study should be a starting point for future research on the long-term effects in natural settings. Furthermore, the proposed tasks in the present study allow only one reaction by the participant but no turn taking as it is often the case in SMC. Thus, to augment ecological validity, future research should extend the duration of SMC and participants to respond to a manipulated response. Thematic analysis of such responses would then allow to deduct different forms of reactions that are manifestations of perceptions of social presence, trust, and support, investigated in the present study.

Another idea for future research is the extension of the current project to many-to-many SMC as it has been done in the longitudinal study by Walther [95] on computer-mediated group interactions to analyze different relational communication dimensions. Transferred to the context of SMC, it would be interesting to investigate synchronicity, modality, and message valence used by participants during a group chat on different topics initiated by the researcher over the course of several weeks. The use of ecological momentary assessment evaluations [96] on emotional state and social presence would allow to collect detailed information on the subjective effects of the interaction without taking participants out of their everyday environment.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Experimental material for both synchronous (immediate) and asynchronous (7 minutes delay) conditions: a) = with emojis, positive answers, b) text only, positive answers, c) with emojis, negative answers, d) text only, negative answers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237846.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Chiara Antonietti and Chiara Filipponi for their help during data collection.

References

  1. 1. Statista. Forecast of the smartphone user penetration rate in Switzerland from 2015 to 2022. 2020. https://www.statista.com/statistics/568277/predicted-smartphone-user-penetration-rate-in-switzerland/
    • 2. Ariel Y, Elishar-Malka V, Avidar R, Levy E-C. Smartphone usage among young Israeli adults: a combined quantitative and qualitative approach. Israel Affairs. 2017;23: 970–986.
      • View Article
      • Google Scholar
    • 3. Flanagin AJ. IM Online: Instant Messaging Use Among College Students. Communication Research Reports. 2005: 175–187.
      • 4. Bakker GD, Sloep P, Jochems W. Students and instant messaging: a survey of current use and demands for higher education. Research in Learning Technology. 2007;15.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 5. Cho J. Roles of Smartphone App Use in Improving Social Capital and Reducing Social Isolation. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2015;18: 350–355. pmid:26075923
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 6. Schober MF, Conrad FG, Antoun C, Ehlen P, Fail S, Hupp AL, et al. Precision and Disclosure in Text and Voice Interviews on Smartphones. Ioerger TR, editor. PLoS ONE. 2015;10: e0128337. pmid:26060991
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 7. Park EK, Sundar SS. Can synchronicity and visual modality enhance social presence in mobile messaging? Computers in Human Behavior. 2015;45: 121–128.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 8. Chan DK-S, Cheng GH-L. A Comparison of Offline and Online Friendship Qualities at Different Stages of Relationship Development. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2004;21: 305–320.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 9. Friend C, Fox Hamilton N. Deception Detection: The Relationship of Levels of Trust and Perspective Taking in Real-Time Online and Offline Communication Environments. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016;19: 532–537. pmid:27635441
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 10. Nguyen M, Bin YS, Campbell A. Comparing Online and Offline Self-Disclosure: A Systematic Review. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2012;15: 103–111. pmid:22032794
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 11. Pew Research Center. Teens, Social Media & Technology. 2018. http://www.pewinternet.org/2018/05/31/teens-social-media-technology-2018/
        • 12. Preece J, Maloney-Krichmar D, Abras C. History of Online Communities. Christensen Karen & Levinson David (Eds), Encyclopedia of Community: From Village to Virtual World. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2003. pp. 1023–1027.
          • 13. Bianchi A, Phillips JG. Psychological Predictors of Problem Mobile Phone Use. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 2005;8: 39–51. pmid:15738692
            • View Article
            • PubMed/NCBI
            • Google Scholar
          • 14. Kim Y, Wang Y, Oh J. Digital Media Use and Social Engagement: How Social Media and Smartphone Use Influence Social Activities of College Students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016;19: 264–269. pmid:26991638
            • View Article
            • PubMed/NCBI
            • Google Scholar
          • 15. Liu D, Yang C. Media Niche of Electronic Communication Channels in Friendship: A Meta-Analysis: MEDIA NICHE OF COMMUNICATION CHANNELS. J Comput-Mediat Comm. 2016;21: 451–466.
            • View Article
            • Google Scholar
          • 16. Dimmick J, Feaster JC, Ramirez A. The niches of interpersonal media: Relationships in time and space. New Media & Society. 2011;13: 1265–1282.
            • View Article
            • Google Scholar
          • 17. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Social Consequences of the Internet for Adolescents: A Decade of Research. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18: 1–5.
            • View Article
            • Google Scholar
          • 18. Kim J-H. Smartphone-mediated communication vs. face-to-face interaction: Two routes to social support and problematic use of smartphone. Computers in Human Behavior. 2017;67: 282–291.
            • View Article
            • Google Scholar
          • 19. Hjorth L. Studying Mobile Media: Cultural Technologies, Mobile Communication, and the iPhone. 1st ed. Routledge; 2012.
            • 20. Daft RL, Lengel RH. Organizational Information Requirements, Media Richness and Structural Design. Management Science. 1986;32: 554–571.
              • View Article
              • Google Scholar
            • 21. Dennis AR, Kinney ST. Testing Media Richness Theory in the New Media: The Effects of Cues, Feedback, and Task Equivocality. Information Systems Research. 1998;9: 256–274.
              • View Article
              • Google Scholar
            • 22. An J, Li T, Teng Y, Zhang P. Factors influencing emoji usage in smartphone mediated communications. In: Springer; 2018.
              • 23. Lowenthal PR. Social presence: What is it? How do we measure it? Doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado Denver, Denver, Colorado. 2012.
                • 24. Short J, Williams E, Christie B. The social psychology of telecommunications. London; New York: Wiley; 1976.
                  • 25. Cui G, Lockee B, Meng C. Building modern online social presence: A review of social presence theory and its instructional design implications for future trends. Educ Inf Technol. 2013;18: 661–685.
                    • View Article
                    • Google Scholar
                  • 26. Biocca F, Harms C, Burgoon JK. Toward a More Robust Theory and Measure of Social Presence: Review and Suggested Criteria. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments. 2003;12: 456–480.
                    • View Article
                    • Google Scholar
                  • 27. Tu C-H. How Chinese Perceive Social Presence: An Examination of Interaction in Online Learning Environment. Educational Media International. 2001;38: 45–60.
                    • View Article
                    • Google Scholar
                  • 28. Gunawardena CN, Zittle FJ. Social presence as a predictor of satisfaction within a computer‐mediated conferencing environment. American Journal of Distance Education. 1997;11: 8–26.
                    • View Article
                    • Google Scholar
                  • 29. Rourke L, Anderson T, Garrison DR, Arher W. Methodological issues in the content analysis of computer conference transcripts. 2011. pp. 8–22.
                    • 30. Tu C-H, McIsaac M. The Relationship of Social Presence and Interaction in Online Classes. American Journal of Distance Education. 2002;16: 131–150.
                      • View Article
                      • Google Scholar
                    • 31. Ptaszynski M, Rzepka R, Araki K, Momouchi Y. Research on emoticons: review of the field and proposal of research framework. 2011. pp. 1159–1162.
                      • 32. Derks D, Bos AER, von Grumbkow J. Emoticons in Computer-Mediated Communication: Social Motives and Social Context. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 2008;11: 99–101. pmid:18275321
                        • View Article
                        • PubMed/NCBI
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 33. Ganster T, Eimler SC, Krämer NC. Same Same But Different!? The Differential Influence of Smilies and Emoticons on Person Perception. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2012;15: 226–230. pmid:22394421
                        • View Article
                        • PubMed/NCBI
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 34. Prada M, Rodrigues DL, Garrido MV, Lopes D, Cavalheiro B, Gaspar R. Motives, frequency and attitudes toward emoji and emoticon use. Telematics and Informatics. 2018;35: 1925–1934.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 35. Marengo D, Giannotta F, Settanni M. Assessing personality using emoji: An exploratory study. Personality and Individual Differences. 2017;112: 74–78.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 36. Derks D, Fischer AH, Bos AER. The role of emotion in computer-mediated communication: A review. Computers in Human Behavior. 2008;24: 766–785.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 37. Garrison DR, Anderson T, Archer W. Critical Inquiry in a Text-Based Environment: Computer Conferencing in Higher Education. The Internet and Higher Education. 1999;2: 87–105.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 38. Felnhofer A, Kothgassner OD, Hauk N, Beutl L, Hlavacs H, Kryspin-Exner I. Physical and social presence in collaborative virtual environments: Exploring age and gender differences with respect to empathy. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014;31: 272–279.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 39. Nicovich SG, Boller GW, Cornwell TB. Experienced Presence within Computer-Mediated Communications: Initial Explorations on the Effects of Gender with Respect to Empathy and Immersion. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2006;10: 00–00.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 40. Sas C, O’Hare GMP. Presence Equation: An Investigation into Cognitive Factors Underlying Presence. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments. 2003;12: 523–537.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 41. Ickes W. Empathic accuracy. In: Kazdin AE, editor. Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol 3. Washington: American Psychological Association; 2000. pp. 177–179.
                        • 42. Ickes W, Stinson L, Bissonnette V, Garcia S. Naturalistic social cognition: Empathic accuracy in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59: 730–742.
                          • View Article
                          • Google Scholar
                        • 43. Sened H, Lavidor M, Lazarus G, Bar-Kalifa E, Rafaeli E, Ickes W. Empathic accuracy and relationship satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Family Psychology. 2017;31: 742–752. pmid:28394141
                          • View Article
                          • PubMed/NCBI
                          • Google Scholar
                        • 44. Bates CE, Samp JA. Examining the Effects of Planning and Empathic Accuracy on Communication in Relational and Nonrelational Conflict Interactions. Communication Studies. 2011;62: 207–223.
                          • View Article
                          • Google Scholar
                        • 45. Baños RM, Botella C, Alcañiz M, Liaño V, Guerrero B, Rey B. Immersion and Emotion: Their Impact on the Sense of Presence. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 2004;7: 734–741. pmid:15687809
                          • View Article
                          • PubMed/NCBI
                          • Google Scholar
                        • 46. Society Internet. Why privacy matters. 2017. http://www.internetsociety.org/what-we-do/internet-technology-matters/privacy-identity
                          • 47. Rotter JB. Generalized expectancies for interpersonal trust. American Psychologist. 1971;26: 443–452.
                            • View Article
                            • Google Scholar
                          • 48. Rotter JB. Interpersonal trust, trustworthiness, and gullibility. American Psychologist. 1980;35: 1–7.
                            • View Article
                            • Google Scholar
                          • 49. Rempel JK, Holmes JG, Zanna MP. Trust in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;49: 95–112.
                            • View Article
                            • Google Scholar
                          • 50. Bos N, Olson J, Gergle D, Olson G, Wright Z. Effects of four computer-mediated communications channels on trust development. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human factors in computing systems Changing our world, changing ourselves—CHI ‘02. Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA: ACM Press; 2002.
                            • 51. Petrocchi S, Filipponi C, Antonietti C, Levante A, Lecciso F. Theory of Mind as a Mediator Between Emotional Trust Beliefs and Interpersonal Communication Competence in a Group of Young Adults. Psychol Rep. 2020; 003329412091348. pmid:32249665
                              • View Article
                              • PubMed/NCBI
                              • Google Scholar
                            • 52. Ji L, Liu J-G, Hou L, Guo Q. Identifying the Role of Common Interests in Online User Trust Formation. Zhang Z-K, editor. PLoS ONE. 2015;10: e0121105. pmid:26161853
                              • View Article
                              • PubMed/NCBI
                              • Google Scholar
                            • 53. Rocco E. Trust breaks down in electronic contexts but can be repaired by some initial face-to-face contact. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human factors in computing systems—CHI ‘98. Los Angeles, California, United States: ACM Press; 1998. pp. 496–502.
                              • 54. Zheng J, Veinott E, Bos N, Olson JS, Olson GM. Trust without touch: jumpstarting long-distance trust with initial social activities. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems. 2002. pp. 141–146.
                                • 55. Gefen D, Straub DW. Consumer trust in B2C e-Commerce and the importance of social presence: experiments in e-Products and e-Services. Omega. 2004;32: 407–424.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 56. Ogonowski A, Montandon A, Botha E, Reyneke M. Should new online stores invest in social presence elements? The effect of social presence on initial trust formation. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services. 2014;21: 482–491.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 57. Bente G, Rüggenberg S, Krämer NC, Eschenburg F. Avatar-Mediated Networking: Increasing Social Presence and Interpersonal Trust in Net-Based Collaborations. Human Comm Res. 2008;34: 287–318.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 58. Cyr D, Hassanein K, Head M, Ivanov A. The role of social presence in establishing loyalty in e-Service environments. Interacting with Computers. 2007;19: 43–56.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 59. Feng J, Lazar J, Preece J. Empathy and online interpersonal trust: A fragile relationship. Behaviour & Information Technology. 2004;23: 97–106.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 60. Rotenberg KJ, Addis N, Betts LR, Corrigan A, Fox C, Hobson Z, et al. The Relation Between Trust Beliefs and Loneliness During Early Childhood, Middle Childhood, and Adulthood. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2010;36: 1086–1100. pmid:20585058
                                  • View Article
                                  • PubMed/NCBI
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 61. Rotenberg KJ, Petrocchi S, Lecciso F, Marchetti A. The Relation Between Children’s Trust Beliefs and Theory of Mind Abilities: Trust Beliefs and Theory of Mind. Inf Child Dev. 2015;24: 206–214.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 62. Simpson JA. Psychological Foundations of Trust. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16: 264–268.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 63. Cutrona CE, Russell D. The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. Jones W H & Perlman D (Eds) Advances in personal relationships. CT: JAI Press; 1987. pp. 37–67.
                                  • 64. Cheung CMK, Chiu P-Y, Lee MKO. Online social networks: Why do students use facebook? Computers in Human Behavior. 2011;27: 1337–1343.
                                    • View Article
                                    • Google Scholar
                                  • 65. Nowak KL. Choosing Buddy Icons that look like me or represent my personality: Using Buddy Icons for social presence. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29: 1456–1464.
                                    • View Article
                                    • Google Scholar
                                  • 66. Walther JB, Van Der Heide B, Hamel LM, Shulman HC. Self-Generated Versus Other-Generated Statements and Impressions in Computer-Mediated Communication: A Test of Warranting Theory Using Facebook. Communication Research. 2009;36: 229–253.
                                    • View Article
                                    • Google Scholar
                                  • 67. Back MD, Schmukle SC, Egloff B. How extraverted is ? Inferring personality from e-mail addresses. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42: 1116–1122.
                                    • View Article
                                    • Google Scholar
                                  • 68. Back MD, Schmukle SC, Egloff B. Why are narcissists so charming at first sight? Decoding the narcissism–popularity link at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010;98: 132–145. pmid:20053038
                                    • View Article
                                    • PubMed/NCBI
                                    • Google Scholar
                                  • 69. McCRAE RR, Costa PT, Jr. Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective. Abingdon, UK: Taylor & Francis; 2003.
                                    • 70. Fletcher GJ, Grigg F, Bull V. The organization and accuracy of personality impressions: Neophytes versus experts in trait attribution. New Zealand Journal of Psychology. 198817: 68–77.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 71. Wellman HM, Bartsch K. Young children’s reasoning about beliefs. Cognition. 1988;30: 239–277. pmid:3215003
                                      • View Article
                                      • PubMed/NCBI
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 72. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1971;17: 124–129. pmid:5542557
                                      • View Article
                                      • PubMed/NCBI
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 73. Wallbott HG. Decoding Emotions from Facial Expression: Recent Developments and Findings. European Review of Social Psychology. 1998;9: 191–232.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 74. Hamilton DL. Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior. 1st ed. Psychology Press; 2015.
                                      • 75. Uleman JS, Adil Saribay S, Gonzalez CM. Spontaneous Inferences, Implicit Impressions, and Implicit Theories. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59: 329–360. pmid:17854284
                                        • View Article
                                        • PubMed/NCBI
                                        • Google Scholar
                                      • 76. Frederickx S, Hofmans J. The Role of Personality in the Initiation of Communication Situations. Journal of Individual Differences. 2014;35: 30–37.
                                        • View Article
                                        • Google Scholar
                                      • 77. Peabody D, Goldberg LR. Some determinants of factor structures from personality-trait descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989;57: 552–567. pmid:2778639
                                        • View Article
                                        • PubMed/NCBI
                                        • Google Scholar
                                      • 78. Anderson CA. Motivational and performance deficits in interpersonal settings: The effect of attributional style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;45: 1136–1147.
                                        • View Article
                                        • Google Scholar
                                      • 79. Savicki V, Kelley M. Computer Mediated Communication: Gender and Group Composition. CyberPsychology & Behavior. 2000;3: 817–826.
                                        • View Article
                                        • Google Scholar
                                      • 80. Park N, Lee H. Gender Difference in Social Networking on Smartphones: A Case Study of Korean College Student Smartphone Users. International Telecommunications Policy Review. 201421: 1–18.
                                        • 81. Novak KP, Smailović J, Sluban B, Mozetič I. Sentiment of Emojis. Perc M, editor. PLoS ONE. 2015;10: e0144296. pmid:26641093
                                          • View Article
                                          • PubMed/NCBI
                                          • Google Scholar
                                        • 82. Wood I, Ruder S. Emoji as emotion tags for tweets. In Proceedings of the Emotion and Sentiment Analysis. In Proceedings of the Emotion and Sentiment Analysis Workshop LREC2016. Portorož, Slovenia: ELRA—European Language Resources Association; 2016. pp. 76–79. http://lrec2016.lrec-conf.org/media/filer_public/2016/06/14/lrec2016-boa-ws.pdf
                                          • 83. Osgood CE. Semantic Differential Technique in the Comparative Study of Cultures1. American Anthropologist. 2009;66: 171–200.
                                            • View Article
                                            • Google Scholar
                                          • 84. Burgoon JK, Hale JL. Validation and measurement of the fundamental themes of relational communication. Communication Monographs. 1987;54: 19–41.
                                            • View Article
                                            • Google Scholar
                                          • 85. Schulz U, Schwarzer R. Soziale Unterstützung bei der Krankheitsbewältigung: Die Berliner Social Support Skalen (BSSS). Diagnostica. 2003;49: 73–82.
                                            • View Article
                                            • Google Scholar
                                          • 86. Ashton MC, Lee K, Goldberg LR. The IPIP–HEXACO scales: An alternative, public-domain measure of the personality constructs in the HEXACO model. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;42: 1515–1526.
                                            • View Article
                                            • Google Scholar
                                          • 87. IBM Corp. Armonk, NY; 2016.
                                            • 88. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: Latent Variable Analysis. R package version 0.3–3. 2011.
                                              • 89. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2018. https://www.R-project.org/
                                                • 90. Hancock JT, Dunham PJ. Impression Formation in Computer-Mediated Communication Revisited: An Analysis of the Breadth and Intensity of Impressions. Communication Research. 2001;28: 325–347.
                                                  • View Article
                                                  • Google Scholar
                                                • 91. Bryant J, Oliver MB, editors. Media effects: advances in theory and research. 3rd ed. New York: Routledge; 2009.
                                                  • 92. Fahr A. Fernsehen fühlen. Ein Ansatz zur Messung von Rezeptionsemotionen. Wirth Werner, Schramm Holger & Gehrau Volker (Hrsg), Unterhaltung durch Medien: Theorien und Messung. Köln: Herbert von Halem; 2006. p. S. 204–226.
                                                    • 93. Ravaja N. Contributions of Psychophysiology to Media Research: Review and Recommendations. Media Psychology. 2004;6: 193–235.
                                                      • View Article
                                                      • Google Scholar
                                                    • 94. Childwise. Monitor Report. A comprehensive annual report focused on children and young people’s media consumption, purchasing habits, attitudes and activities. CHILDWISE Monitor Report 2020. 2020. http://www.childwise.co.uk/
                                                      • 95. Walther JB. Relational Aspects of Computer-Mediated Communication: Experimental Observations over Time. Organization Science. 1995;6: 186–203.
                                                        • View Article
                                                        • Google Scholar
                                                      • 96. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological Momentary Assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4: 1–32. pmid:18509902
                                                        • View Article
                                                        • PubMed/NCBI
                                                        • Google Scholar

                                                      What refers to the delay between the time from when a message is sent to when it is received?

                                                      Propagation delay is the amount of time required for a signal to be received after it has been sent; it is caused by the time it takes for the signal to travel through a medium.

                                                      What is synchronous and asynchronous with example?

                                                      The key difference between synchronous and asynchronous communication is synchronous communications are scheduled, real-time interactions by phone, video, or in-person. Asynchronous communication happens on your own time and doesn't need scheduling.

                                                      What are the examples of asynchronous?

                                                      In a nutshell, asynchronous communication is any communication that does not take place in real-time. Emails, forum comments, corporate intranet, and even Asana or Trello boards serve as examples of asynchronous communication we deal with every day.

                                                      What is the meaning of persistence and synchronicity in the context of message oriented communication?

                                                      persistent vs transient communication, asynchronous communication – sender continues immediately after it has sub- mitted its message for transmission, synchronous communication – the sender blocked until its message is stored in a local buffer at the receiving host or actually delivered to the receiver.